Search

Do Sharp and Flat Keys Evoke Opposing Sensory Perception? : Focusing on the Crossmodal Influence Inherent in the Pitch and Characteristics of Enharmonic Key

Authors_Previous
External Authors
Fields
Journal / Conference
한국서양음악학회
서양음악학
Status
Year
2022
Authors
Related to Ph.D. Students (Related to Publications (Authors) 1)
Related to Ph.D. Students (Related to Publications (Authors) 2)
Related to Ph.D. Students (Related to Publications (Authors))
Related to Alumni (Related to Publications (Authors))
Related to people merged (Related to Publications (Authors))
Related to Untitled Database (Publications (Author)에 관계됨)
Related to Untitled Database (Publications (Author)에 관계됨) 1
Semester
제목 없는 데이터베이스 (Publications (Author)에 관계됨)에 관계됨

Authors : Ahyeon Choi(chah0623@snu.ac.kr), Woojae Cho, and Kyogu Lee

Keywords : musical keys, sharp and flat, sensory perception, enharmonic, brightness, warmth, and sharpness

Background

A wide spectrum of elements in music such as pitch contours, rhythm, harmony, tempo, and dynamics affect emotions or senses that the music evokes. In particular, the musical key has been considered a crucial attribute in determining the overall atmosphere of music. Previous studies have shown that the major key is related to brightness and happiness, while the minor key is related to darkness and sadness. However, keys also can be subdivided into sharp (#) keys and flat (b) keys. Sharp and flat keys are constructed in opposite directions in the Circle of Fifth, which is known to be musically associated with tension and relaxation. Thus, sharp and flat keys are likely to cause opposing sensory perceptions in music.

Aims

Based on these hypotheses, we investigated whether sharp and flat keys of music indeed have different effects on the perception of three senses: brightness, warmth, and sharpness.

Methods

A total of 35 musicians (Mage= 22, Mtrainingyears = 8.4) responded to their perceived senses after listening to Mozart’s piano sonatas where the key is established in eight bars.
In session 1, K.545 and K.310 were transposed into 24 different keys, and in session 2, K.279, K.309, and K.330 were transposed into only the enharmonic keys. All stimuli were randomly presented as a score-recording pair in each session, and then participants responded with a 7-point scale for brightness, warmth, and sharpness at the end of each stimulus.

Results

To analyze the results of session 1, we perform linear regression between the keys(X) and the perceived sensory scores(Y) from the key. Consistent with previous studies showing that pitch height affects the perception of brightness, warmth, and sharpness, X and Y show linearity along with the height of key’s pitch in all senses (mean of r² = 0.839, sd = 0.099). However, the scatter plot allows us to see that sharp and flat keys are perceived as opposite senses. Sharp keys are perceived much brighter, cooler, and sharper, and flat keys are perceived much darker, warmer, and flatter.
These tendencies are better demonstrated in session 2, which consists only of enharmonic keys with the same actual pitches but different sharp and flat key signatures. The paired t-test shows that perceived sensory scores for sharp and flat keys are significantly different in all three senses (p < 0.05), with the same aspect as the result of session 1.

Conclusion and Implication

These results suggest that even with the same music, we might perceive different senses depending on whether the key is sharp or flat.

Reference

Cohen, A. J. (1991). Tonality and perception: Musical scales primed by excerpts from The Well-Tempered Clavier of JS Bach. Psychological Research, 53(4), 305-314.
Isbilen, E. S., & Krumhansl, C. L. (2016). The color of music: Emotion-mediated associations to Bach’s Well-tempered Clavier. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 26(2), 149.
Krumhansl, C. L. (2004). The cognition of tonality–as we know it today. Journal of New Music Research, 33(3), 253-268.
Itoh, K., Sakata, H., Kwee, I. L., & Nakada, T. (2017). Musical pitch classes have rainbow hues in pitch class-color synesthesia. Scientific reports, 7(1), 1-9.
Palmer, S. E., Schloss, K. B., Xu, Z., & Prado-León, L. R. (2013). Music–color associations are mediated by emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(22), 8836-8841.
Parncutt, R. (2014). The emotional connotations of major versus minor tonality: One or more origins?. Musicae Scientiae, 18(3), 324-353.
Powell, J., & Dibben, N. (2005). Key-mood association: A self perpetuating myth. Musicae Scientiae, 9(2), 289-311.
Virtala, P., Berg, V., Kivioja, M., Purhonen, J., Salmenkivi, M., Paavilainen, P., & Tervaniemi, M. (2011). The preattentive processing of major vs. minor chords in the human brain: An event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters, 487(3), 406-410.
Virtala, P., & Tervaniemi, M. (2017). Neurocognition of major-minor and consonance-dissonance. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 34(4), 387-404.